Share to: share facebook share twitter share wa share telegram print page

N2pc

N2pc refers to an ERP component linked to selective attention.[1] The N2pc appears over visual cortex contralateral to the location in space to which subjects are attending; if subjects pay attention to the left side of the visual field, the N2pc appears in the right hemisphere of the brain, and vice versa. This characteristic makes it a useful tool for directly measuring the general direction of a person's attention (either left or right) with fine-grained temporal resolution.

History

Luck and Hillyard (1990) [2] first observed the N2pc while seeking to document electrophysiological correlates of focused attention during visual search using ERPs. Subjects viewed arrays containing 4-12 items, one of which was a target on 50% of trials. Compared to the waveform over cortex ipsilateral to the target, experimenters observed a consistently greater negative deflection of the ERP waveform at approximately 200 ms after the stimulus at posterior sites (i.e., over visual cortex) contralateral to the side of the screen subjects attended.

The N2pc first received its name from Luck and Hillyard (1994),[3] who named the component after its characteristic features. The "N" denotes a negative polarity; "2" describes its latency in the waveform (i.e., the second negative deflection, typically around 200 ms); and "pc" stands for "posterior-contralateral," as the component appears over posterior electrode sites contralateral to the direction of attention. The experimenters explored what factors would modulate the N2pc using a visual search paradigm in which subjects had to report the presence of a target object in a display (e.g., a green box or a horizontal bar). They confirmed that the N2pc appeared contralateral to attended stimuli, and furthermore found that it did not appear when subjects saw only one object at a time or had to spread their attention over all the items in the display. These data led the experimenters to believe the N2pc corresponds to a filtering process that occurs whenever people focus attention on one object while ignoring others.

Component characteristics

The component's name, N2pc, abbreviates its characteristics.[3] The component belongs to the family of N2 ERP components, a negative deflection in the ERP waveform at a latency of approximately 200-300 ms following a stimulus. The "pc" stands for "posterior-contralateral", describing the topographic distribution of the component. It appears as a greater negativity at posterior electrode sites contralateral to the attended side of the visual field relative to ipsilateral electrode sites. For example, when a person pays attention to something in the left side of the visual field, an N2pc appears as a greater negativity over the right posterior areas of the brain than the left. MEG has been used to localize the N2pc primarily to lateral extrastriate cortex and inferotemporal visual areas, such as V4.[4]

Classic paradigms and findings

The N2pc can be used flexibly in nearly any task in which one would like to study the direction and time course of selective attention. However, researchers have primarily used the N2pc in visual search paradigms to study the deployment of attention over time and test hypotheses of parallel and serial models of visual search.[5]

The first experiments to investigate specifically the N2pc used a visual search paradigm in which subjects reported the presence or absence of a pre-defined target (e.g., a green rectangle) in a display containing one "oddball" stimulus that differed on a single feature from a uniform background of items (e.g., a green square among blue squares). The oddball stimuli would "pop out" and attract attention, but were not necessarily targets. As a result, experimenters knew where subjects directed attention, but could simultaneously manipulate factors orthogonal to the location of attention, such as low-level features or probability of the target appearing.[6] The pop-out oddball would generate an N2pc, as it received focused attention, while stimulus characteristics modulated the amplitude and latency of the component.

Subsequent investigation into the N2pc manipulated the number of items in the array and found that a display with as few as two objects elicits the component. Because an object cannot "pop out" and attract attention in a two-item display, experimenters concluded that the N2pc must reflect top-down, controlled processes of directing attention. The same study also demonstrated that the N2pc does not only occur when attending to visual features, but semantic features as well. In one experiment, subjects had to respond to the words "left" and "right" while ignoring the color words "white" and "brown." Even in this case of semantically defined targets, subjects demonstrated an N2pc contralateral to the target word. Together, these results have provided strong evidence that the N2pc reflects the location of covert, consciously directed attention.[7]

The prototypical visual search paradigm for eliciting an N2pc component has subjects attend and respond to a target stimulus to the left or right of fixation. Unlike regular visual search experiments, however, two major criteria most hold when attempting to measure N2pc response. First, the stimuli should be identical in all conditions, and the experimenter should only manipulate instructions for directing attention across conditions; this precludes the possibility that stimulus features drive ERP effects rather than focused attention. Second, the target should be easy to find, usually via "pop-out." The goal is to minimize the variability in search times and N2pc latency, resulting in a much clearer signal when the waveforms are averaged together over multiple trials.[1]

An example experiment for eliciting the N2pc that follows the critical principles above might proceed as follows: Subjects see an array of upright and inverted T's. One T is red, and one T is green, but the rest are black (thus fulfilling the first criterion of easy-to-find targets). Subjects are told to attend to either the red T's or the green T's at the beginning of the experiment and report whether that letter is upright or inverted (thus fulfilling the second criterion that attention should not be confounded with stimulus characteristics). We should expect to see an N2pc contralateral to the side of the screen the attended letter appeared.

Functional sensitivity

Amplitude

The N2pc is primarily sensitive to the directional focus of attention over time. However, research has found a variety of factors that modulate N2pc response. N2pc amplitude is sensitive to factors related to increasing demands on focused attention.

  • When non-target stimuli closely resemble the target (e.g., when targets are defined by size, but the size difference between targets and distractors is very small), they elicit an N2pc of lower amplitude than a target stimulus[3][6]
  • When targets are defined by a conjunction of features (e.g., blue, horizontal bar) rather than a single feature (e.g., blue bar), they elicit a larger N2pc,[8] which may reflect a greater demand on attention to identify the target.[9]
  • In a dual-task situation where subjects focus on a demanding primary task while performing target detection as a secondary task, the N2pc only appears in response to detecting targets defined by conjunctions of features. Again, the greater attentional demands of conjunction-based targets relative to feature-based targets may be responsible.[10]
  • N2pc amplitude increases as distractors appear closer to the target, which increases the need to focus on the target while filtering the distractors (but also see Mazza et al., 2009,[11] who found conflicting results).
  • When subjects have to indicate where a target is located, they exhibit a larger N2pc than when they simply have to report whether or not a target is present[8]

Elimination

Certain experimental conditions can eliminate the N2pc entirely. These results have been used to argue for a spatial filtering hypothesis, which proposes that the N2pc reflects the process of ignoring task-irrelevant (i.e., non-target) stimuli.

Early investigations of the N2pc critically found that the component was sensitive to the presence of distractors,[6] appearing only when distractors accompanied a target stimulus. Furthermore, N2pc amplitude increases with the number of distractors in the display.[11]

The N2pc also disappears when targets in the visual search task are defined as "any oddball object" rather than by one or more specific features. Luck and Hillyard (1994)[3] have argued that in this case, determining whether a given object is a target requires distributing attention over multiple objects in the array (and determining the common features) rather than filtering them. Consequently, the spatial filtering process is discouraged, and the N2pc therefore does not appear.

Functional significance

The N2pc literature agrees on a few functional characteristics of the N2pc. First, the N2pc appears whenever a person focuses attention on an object. Second, it serves as a direct measure for the direction of focused attention, either to the left or right. Finally, the N2pc is generally believed to be tied to a spatial filtering hypothesis (see above: "Eliminating the N2pc"). The last point regarding the functional significance of the N2pc, however, has been challenged. Some have contested the spatial filtering hypothesis, arguing that the N2pc reflects an enhancement of task-relevant stimulus processing rather than a suppression of irrelevant stimuli.[11]

Other work has explored further cognitive processes that could be linked to the N2pc. For instance, the classic visual search paradigm that elicits the N2pc could be broken down further into processes of shifting attention, and spatially based processing of non-target locations. When combining the visual search task with visual cues that drew attention to spatial locations in the display, experimenters found that while the N2pc may not reflect shifts of attention, it may still reflect processing of a location in space that may or may not contain a target.[12]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b Luck, S. J. (2005). "The operation of attention—millisecond by millisecond—over the first half second." In H. Ogmen & B. G. Breitmeyer (Eds.), The first half second: The microgenesis and temporal dynamics of unconscious and conscious visual processes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
  2. ^ Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1990). Electrophysiological evidence for parallel and serial processing during visual search. Perception & Psychophysics, 48, 603-617.
  3. ^ a b c d Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. Psychophysiology 31, 291-308.
  4. ^ Hopf, J.-M.; Luck, S. J.; Girelli, M.; Hagner, T.; Mangun, G. R.; Scheich, H.; Heinze, H. J. (2000). "Neural sources of focused attention in visual search". Cerebral Cortex. 10 (12): 1233–1241. doi:10.1093/cercor/10.12.1233. PMID 11073872.
  5. ^ Woodman, G. F.; Luck, S. J. (2003). "Serial deployment of attention during visual search". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 29: 121–138. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.457.3225. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.29.1.121.
  6. ^ a b c Luck, S. J.; Hillyard, S. A. (1994). "Spatial filtering during visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 20 (5): 1000–1014. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.5.1000.
  7. ^ Eimer, M. (1996). "The N2pc as an indicator of attentional selectivity". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 99 (3): 225–234. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.545.141. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(96)95711-9. PMID 8862112.
  8. ^ a b Luck, S.J.; Girelli, M.; McDermott, M.T.; Ford, M.A. (1997). "Bridging the gap between monkey neurophysiology and human perception: An ambiguity resolution theory of visual selective attention". Cognitive Psychology. 33 (1): 64–87. doi:10.1006/cogp.1997.0660. PMID 9212722. S2CID 11065167.
  9. ^ Treisman, A.M.; Gelade, G. (1980). "A feature-integration theory of attention". Cognitive Psychology. 12 (1): 97–136. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5. PMID 7351125. S2CID 353246.
  10. ^ Luck, S. J.; Ford, M. A. (1998). "On the role of selective attention in visual perception". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 95 (3): 825–830. Bibcode:1998PNAS...95..825L. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.3.825. PMC 33804. PMID 9448247.
  11. ^ a b c Mazza, V.; Turatto, M.; Caramazza, A. (2009). "Attention selection, distractor suppression, and N2pc". Cortex. 45 (7): 879–890. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.10.009. PMID 19084218. S2CID 10575482.
  12. ^ Kiss, M.; Van Velzen, J.; Eimer, M. (2008). "The N2pc component and its links to attention shifts and spatially selective visual processing". Psychophysiology. 45 (2): 240–249. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00611.x. PMC 2248220. PMID 17971061.
Read more information:

I'm 10,000 Years oldAlbum studio karya Elvis PresleyDirilis2 Januari 1971DirekamJuni dan September 1970GenreCountry, country rock, rock and rollDurasi38:49LabelRCA RecordsProduserFelton JarvisKronologi Elvis Presley That's the Way It Is(1970)That's the Way It Is1970 Elvis Country (I'm 10,000 Years Old)(1971) You'll Never Walk Alone(1971)You'll Never Walk Alone1971 Singel dalam album Elvis Country I Really Don't Want to KnowDirilis: 8 Desember 1970 Penilaian profesional Skor ulasan Sumber Nil…

Untuk kegunaan lain, lihat Kelambu (disambiguasi). Sebuah tempat tidur yang ditutupi kelambu. kelambu yang memiliki rangka penopang sendiri (self propping mosquito net) Kelambu di rumah orang Belanda di Ulee Lheue, Meuraksa, Banda Aceh (1925) Kelambu memberi perlindungan dari nyamuk, lalat, dan serangga lainnya termasuk penyakit yang disebabkan serangga-serangga tersebut, seperti malaria dan filariasis. Kelambu merupakan sebuah tirai tipis, tembus pandang, dengan jaring-jaring yang dapat menahan…

Anang Murdianto Informasi pribadiLahir10 Agustus 1959 (umur 64)Puger, JemberMeninggal20 Maret 2022(2022-03-20) (umur 62)JakartaKebangsaanIndonesiaAlma materAkademi Angkatan Udara (1983)Karier militerPihak IndonesiaDinas/cabang TNI Angkatan UdaraMasa dinas1983–2017Pangkat Marsekal Muda TNISatuanKorps PenerbangSunting kotak info • L • B Marsekal Muda TNI (Purn.) Anang Murdianto (lahir 10 Agustus 1959) adalah seorang perwira tinggi TNI Angkatan Udara lulusan Akad…

Artikel ini tidak memiliki referensi atau sumber tepercaya sehingga isinya tidak bisa dipastikan. Tolong bantu perbaiki artikel ini dengan menambahkan referensi yang layak. Tulisan tanpa sumber dapat dipertanyakan dan dihapus sewaktu-waktu.Cari sumber: Fetisisme pakaian dalam – berita · surat kabar · buku · cendekiawan · JSTOR Salah satu jenis Fetisisme pakaian dalam adalah Fetisisme terhadap stoking. Fetisisme pakaian dalam adalah fetisisme seksual yang …

MutabbalSajianMakanan pembukaTempat asalTimur TengahBahan utamaTerung, minyak zaitunSunting kotak info • L • BBantuan penggunaan templat ini Buku resep: Mutabbal  Media: Mutabbal Baba ghanoush (Arabic بابا غنوج bābā ghannūj, juga disebut baba ganush, baba ghannouj atau baba ghannoug[1]) adalah masakan khas negeri Syam yang dibuat dari terung yang dimasak, dicampur bawang, tomat, minyak zaitun, dan berbagai bumbu lain. Dalam bahasa Arab baba ghanoush bera…

Drs.Baharuddin Yahya Wali Kota Banda Aceh ke-7Masa jabatan1983 – 1993 PendahuluDjakfar AhmadPenggantiSaid Hussain Al-Haj Sunting kotak info • L • B Teuku Ibrahim adalah Wali Kota Banda Aceh kedelapan setelah menggantikan Djakfar Ahmad dan menjabat pada tahun 1983 sampai 1993. Pada tahun 1993, Teuku Ibrahim digantikan oleh Said Hussain Al-Haj. Jabatan politik Didahului oleh:Djakfar Ahmad Wali kota Banda Aceh1983 - 1993 Diteruskan oleh:Said Hussain Al-Haj

Eduard Herman s'Jacob Eduard Herman s'Jacob (4 Agustus 1827 – 15 April 1912) adalah politikus Belanda. S'Jacob adalah administratur abad ke-19 yang berasal dari keluarga bangsawan Rotterdam, yang duduk dalam 2 kali masa jabatan di Tweede Kamer. EH. s'Jacob adalah putera tokoh konservatif Frederik Bernard s'Jacob. Kakaknya Frederik kelak menjabat sebagai GubJend. Hindia Belanda. Pada tahun 1850, S'Jacob bekerja sebagai pengacara di Hindia Belanda dan setelah kembali ke Belanda akt…

陆军第十四集团军炮兵旅陆军旗存在時期1950年 - 2017年國家或地區 中国效忠於 中国 中国共产党部門 中国人民解放军陆军種類炮兵功能火力支援規模约90门火炮直屬南部战区陆军參與戰役1979年中越战争 中越边境冲突 老山战役 成都军区对越轮战 紀念日10月25日 陆军第十四集团军炮兵旅(英語:Artillery Brigade, 14th Army),是曾经中国人民解放军陆军第十四集团军下属的…

جزء من سلسلة مقالات حولالرسول محمد سيرته سيرته بعثته حياته في مكة هجرته إلى يثرب حياته في المدينة فتح مكة حجة الوداع أحداث وجوانب من حياته نزول الوحي أحاديثه الهجرة إلى الحبشة بيعة العقبة بيعة الرضوان إصلاحاته الاجتماعية دبلوماسيته عسكريته معجزاته القرآن الإسراء والمعراج …

RawalumbuKecamatanNegara IndonesiaProvinsiJawa BaratKotaBekasiPemerintahan • CamatDian Herdiana, A.P., M.Si.Populasi • Total227.198 jiwa (2.014)[1] jiwaKode Kemendagri32.75.05 Kode BPS3275041 Desa/kelurahan4 Rawalumbu adalah sebuah kecamatan yang terletak di Kota Bekasi, Provinsi Jawa Barat, Indonesia. Sejarah Sejarah pembentukan Kecamatan Rawalumbu adalah bagian dari Kecamatan Bekasi Timur, namun seiring dengan berlakunya UU Nomor 22 Tahun 1999 Tentang Otono…

Artikel ini perlu dikembangkan agar dapat memenuhi kriteria sebagai entri Wikipedia.Bantulah untuk mengembangkan artikel ini. Jika tidak dikembangkan, artikel ini akan dihapus. Artikel ini tidak memiliki referensi atau sumber tepercaya sehingga isinya tidak bisa dipastikan. Tolong bantu perbaiki artikel ini dengan menambahkan referensi yang layak. Tulisan tanpa sumber dapat dipertanyakan dan dihapus sewaktu-waktu.Cari sumber: Gerakan Hak Asasi Manusia dan Demokrasi – berita …

João da NovaLahir1460Maceda, GalisiaMeninggalJuli 16, 1509 – 1460; umur -50–-49 tahunKochi, IndiaKebangsaanKerajaan Galisia dan Kerajaan PortugalPekerjaanPenjelajahDikenal atasPenemuan Pulau Ascension dan Saint Helena João da Nova (pengucapan Galisia Xoán de Novoa atau Joam de Nôvoa, pengucapan Spanyol Juan de Nova; pengucapan bahasa Portugis: [ʒuˈɐ̃w̃ dɐ ˈnɔvɐ]; lahir s. 1460 di Maceda, Ourense, Galisia; meninggal pada 16 Juli 1509 di Kochi, India) adalah seor…

Peta Trinidad dan Tobago Untuk kegunaan lain, lihat Tobago. Trinidad adalah pulau terbesar di antara 23 pulau-pulau yang membentuk Republik Trinidad dan Tobago. Pulau paling selatan di kawasan Karibia ini terletak 11 km sebelah timur laut pesisir Venezuela. Luas pulau ini sebesar 4.769 km². Ibu kota negara, Port-of-Spain, terletak di bagian barat laut Trinidad. Sejarah Trinidad awalnya dihuni oleh penduduk pribumi Amerika. Christopher Columbus menemukan pulau ini pada 1498 dan semenjak itu Trin…

President of Mexico from 1928 to 1930 In this Spanish name, the first or paternal surname is Portes and the second or maternal family name is Gil. Emilio Portes GilEmilio Portes Gil, 192848th President of MexicoIn office1 December 1928 – 4 February 1930Preceded byPlutarco Elías CallesSucceeded byPascual Ortiz RubioAttorney General of MexicoIn office5 September 1932 – 30 November 1934PresidentAbelardo L. RodríguezPreceded byJosé Aguilar y MayaSucceeded bySilve…

Americans of Asian ancestry This article is about the U.S. population of Americans of Asian ancestry. For populations of Asians across the Americas, see Asians in the Americas. For the documentary series, see Asian Americans (film series). Asian AmericansDistribution of Asian Americans by countyTotal population 24,009,902 (alone and in combination)7.2% of the population (2020)[1] Chinese Americans: 5,143,982Indian Americans: 4,506,308Filipino Americans: 4,089,570Vietnamese Americans: 2,1…

Cantonese style of barbecued pork Char siuA rack of cha siu porkAlternative nameschasu, char siu, chashao, cha sio, char siew (Cantonese), xá xíu (Vietnamese)Place of originGuangdong, ChinaRegion or stateGreater China, Japan and Singapore (and general Sinophone areas in Southeast Asia and beyond)Main ingredientsPork, mixture of honey, five-spice powder, fermented tofu (red), dark soy sauce, hoisin sauce, and sherry or rice wine Cookbook: Char siu  Media: Char siu Char siuChar siu in T…

Edyta GórniakEdyta Górniak in 2019Lahir14 November 1972 (umur 51)Ziębice, PolandiaPekerjaanSingerSuami/istriDariusz Krupa (cerai)Anak1Karier musikTahun aktif1989–presentLabelEMI Music, Virgin Music, Universal Music PolandSitus webwww.edytagorniak.comTanda tangan Edyta Anna Górniak (lahir 14 November 1972) adalah seorang penyanyi pop Polandia dengan karier selama 3 dekade. Dia terkenal karena memiliki jangkauan vokal yang luas sekitar empat oktaf. Górniak dimulai sebagai aktris teater…

Menteri Pekerjaan Umum MalaysiaMenteri Kerja Raya Malaysia منتري كرجا راي مليسياLambangPetahanaAlexander Nanta Linggisejak 3 Desember 2022Kementerian Pekerjaan UmumGelarYang Berhormat Menteri(Yang Terhormat Menteri)Ditunjuk olehYang di-Pertuan Agong atas rekomendasi Perdana Menteri MalaysiaDibentuk9 Agustus 1955; 68 tahun lalu (1955-08-09)Pejabat pertamaSardon JubirSitus webwww.kkr.gov.my Berikut adalah daftar orang yang pernah menjabat sebagai Menteri Pekerjaan (Mela…

Koordinat: 3°46′N 100°53′E / 3.767°N 100.883°E / 3.767; 100.883 Pekan Bagan Nahkoda Omarباڬن نخودا عمر Geografi StatusMukim Asal namaN/A Negara Malaysia Negeri Selangor DaerahDaerah Sabak Bernam MukimDesa Bagan Nahkoda Omar KeluasanN/A Bagan Nakhoda Omar adalah sebuah desa kecil dan pantai yang terletak di distrik Sabak Bernam, Selangor, Malaysia. Pranala luar Bagan Nakhoda Omar Diarsipkan 2013-02-16 di Wayback Machine. di Tourism Selangor lb…

KakaoTVTipelayanan OTTLangueKoreaPemilikKakao (Kakao Entertainment) KakaoTV adalah Korea Selatan OTT televisi internet, personal internet layanan penyiaran dan pemutar video milik Kakao Entertainment yang diluncurkan pada 16 Juni 2015. Sejarah Pada tanggal 16 Juni 2015, KakaoTV dirilis secara resmi. Awalnya, itu adalah situs web yang mirip dengan Naver TV dengan cara menawarkan siaran streaming dari penyiar afiliasi atau kontrak.[1] Pada tanggal 6 November 2015, Kakao meluncurkan fungsi …

Kembali kehalaman sebelumnya